Working with my chronotype: Better use of a task manager

When I cover chronotypes in Intro Psych, I tell my students about the research that found that employees whose work schedules match their chronotypes have higher work satisfaction (Amini et al., 2021).

I’ve always been a morning person. Even as an adolescent, I routinely awakened at 6am without an alarm. Now, deep into adulthood, I routinely awake around 4:30am. Interestingly, to me anyway, that time had been 5:30am, but my brain seems to have never adjusted after last fall’s time change. So, 4:30am it is.

As a college student, I preferred taking the early morning classes. As a college professor, I preferred teaching the early morning classes. When I changed colleges, the new course schedule was very different. The early morning classes were 50 minutes each and five days a week. The afternoon and evening classes were longer sessions and met twice a week. I had to choose between my chronotype and my preferred teaching schedule. I went with my preferred teaching schedule. It worked out fine. At the beginning of the term, I could muster quite bit of energy for class. As the term wore on, my energy waned, but my students were anticipating a high energy class, so they brought the energy, and that was enough to carry me through. Thanks, everyone!

One term, I taught an evening class that started at the ungodly (for me) hour of 7:30pm. When I covered chronotypes, I asked my students if evening classes were a match or a mismatch for their chronotype. Every student said it was a match. Good for them!

As the new executive director for the Society for the Teaching of Psychology, I have a lot more items on my to-do list–currently 49 recurring tasks and counting–so I needed a more robust tracking system than I’ve ever needed before. (Most of those recurring tasks are monthly or yearly; only a couple are weekly)  For my new task manager, I opted for Todoist, which seems to be working really well. Todoist allows me to tag each task. When I started creating tasks, I was tagging by task type, such as Membership for any task having to do with managing the STP membership database or Website for any tasks having to do with updating pages on the STP website. That was fine, but those tags weren’t feeling particularly useful. If I needed to do it today, did it matter if it was a Membership task or a Website task?

My greatest challenge is not being executive director; it is keeping up with my writing schedule (currently, that means revising my Intro Psych textbook). That’s been a challenge with the 20+ hours I need to devote to my executive director role. What I had been doing was getting all of my small tasks done first thing in the morning so I could leave the rest of the day for writing. Too often, though, those small tasks would eat up my entire morning, and in the afternoon, I was too mentally drained to write. It felt like death by a thousand cuts.

When I was telling a friend (shout out to Ellen Carpenter at Virginia Commonwealth University!) about my tagging system, it hit me that I was using tags all wrong. I really only need two tags: Morning and Afternoon. Because—thanks to my chronotype—I’m mentally stronger in the morning and weaker in the afternoon, any task that is cognitively demanding needs to be tagged as a Morning task. Any task that is lighter needs to be tagged as an Afternoon task. If I look at my to-do list and everything is tagged as Afternoon, then I don’t even bother reading what the tasks are: I go directly to writing. I’ve been really happy with how this working.

The next time you cover chronotypes, consider sharing this time management strategy with your students. They don’t have to use Todoist. Even if students are putting their tasks in their calendars, the morning people can add their cognitively demanding tasks to the morning hours, and the night people can add their cognitively demanding tasks to the evening hours—you know, when I’m already asleep.

Reference

Amini, F., Moosavi, S. M., Rafaiee, R., Nadi Ghara, A., & Babakhanian, M. (2021). Chronotype patterns associated with job satisfaction of shift working healthcare providers. Chronobiology International, 38(4), 526–533. https://doi.org/10.1080/07420528.2020.1869028




Talking to strangers can be hard, but it is worth it

The National Institute on the Teaching of Psychology (NITOP) is later this week.

I’ve previously written (Frantz, 2023):

I’ll confess that well before I retired my primary purpose for attending conferences was to meet with my friends and make new friends. In SPQR: A History of Ancient Rome, the author and Roman historian Mary Beard tells us that Polybius (200 BCE – 118 BCE) supposedly advised a young man, “Never come back from the Forum…until you have made at least one new friend” (Beard, 2016, p. 184)

I was reminded of this recently when I read the (freely available) Working Life essay at the end of the November 22, 2024 issue of Science (Suresh, 2024). The author tells us that while she understood the importance of networking, it terrified her. Part of her terror came from thinking she had to impress people. After having good intentions of networking at a conference, she left the conference with no new connections. In reflecting on her experience, she realized she was focusing on the wrong thing. Rather than trying to find ways to impress, she needed to find ways to connect. Not that that was easy for her, either!

I took every opportunity to attend events, and networking became a nonnegotiable part of my agenda. My palms would sweat and I’d lose my train of thought, stumbling over my words as I tried to piece together coherent sentences. I often ended up rambling, unsure I was making any sense at all. My voice would occasionally tremble and I’d become painfully self-conscious, worried I was coming across as unprepared or awkward. But I kept showing up, determined to push through, hoping eventually it would start to feel natural. And it did (Suresh, 2024).

What a great example of sympathetic nervous system arousal to share with our students. Speaking to strangers can be anxiety-producing, but it gets easier with time.

As Polybius knew, there is tremendous value in making new friends. Almost all –if not all—of the opportunities that I’ve had in my work life I can trace back to a specific person whom I had met. And I’ve had the good fortune of being able to pass along opportunities to others. There is also much joy in having a plethora of friends who text me fun photos—and who, more importantly, tolerate me doing the same.

This year, I will be NITOP, MAESTRO, APS, APA, and ACT. If you’re going to be at any of these, let’s chat. Even if you’re not sure what to say, you say, “In your blog, you said that you wanted to chat.” I’ll take it from there. I promise. I’m looking forward to meeting you!

References

Beard, M. (2016). SPQR: A history of ancient Rome. Profile Books.

Frantz, S. (2023, September 10). Retirement on the near horizon? Some thoughts. Macmillan and BFW Teaching Community. https://community.macmillanlearning.com/t5/psychology-blog/retirement-on-the-near-horizon-some-thoughts/ba-p/19300

Suresh, A. (2024). Connection, not perfection. Science, 386(6724), 934.

**The image that accompanies this post was generated by AI. I was going for an ancient Roman forum. This was AI’s fifth try. The first one included a few people in modern dress and what I am pretty sure was a pterodactyl. In retrospect, I should have used that one.




Does the U.S. have a national dance? The basis for a class activity…of sorts

My wife and I took a cruise in 2024. On the last evening, several members of the crew put on a show. Groups of crew members who shared a home country performed popular national dances. This got me thinking about whether the United States had something that could be considered a national dance. If you’d like, take a few minutes to discuss. Does the U.S. have something that could be a national dance?

Years ago, we had a friend who visited India. The group she was visiting showed her how to do one of their national dances. Afterwards, they asked her to teach her a national dance from the United States. I have spent a lot of time thinking about this. In the moment, I probably would have said we don’t have one. Our friend, however, was quick on her feet. She showed them how to do the Hokey Pokey.

If you don’t know how to do the Hokey Pokey, watch this instructional video.

Now, I’ve had a lot of time to consider this question. For a short time, the Chicken Dance was on my list of U.S. national dance contenders. In terms of folk dances, yes, the Hokey Pokey and the Chicken Dance should be on the list. We should also include the Electric Slide, the Macarena, and the Hustle.

But do any of those rise to the status of a national dance? I don’t think so. But I do have a national dance nominee: Y.M.C.A. by the Village People. Performed in stadiums, at weddings, and just about any other gathering (save funerals and memorial services, although I would not be surprised to hear that someone requested that their attendees perform it). As further evidence of its worthiness as the national dance, the song Y.M.C.A is on the Library of Congress’s National Recording Registry list.

That’s pretty remarkable for a song about men picking up men.

On its surface, Y.M.C.A is a song about wholesome activities, which is certainly what the actual YMCA would prefer we think. The lyrics have plausible deniability. The YMCA does have “many ways to have a good time,” and they do indeed “have everything for young men to enjoy.” Such as basketball and swimming. But the double entendre is hard to miss. The album that features the song is called Cruisin’. And the original music video is worth a watch. At the 2:35, 2:53, and 3:11 marks, they dance on the sidewalk with the Greenwich Village Ramrod bar in the background. If Ramrod sounds like a gay leather bar, you would be right.

And, sure, Macho Man is about getting in shape and In the Navy is about the importance of military service. But back to my nomination for a national dance.

When I first watched the Y.M.C.A music video, I was surprised to discover that the Y.M.C.A. dance had not yet been born. The Y is there, but the other letters are not. That didn’t happen until the Village People performed the song on American Bandstand in January 1979. At the 6:25 mark, Dick Clark cues the audience to do the dance. According to a Spin magazine article, at that particular taping were “a bunch of kids bused in from a cheerleader camp” (Pearlman, 2008). Cheerleaders gave us the dance.

Anyway, performing the Y.M.C.A (national?) dance with a group of strangers gives us about three and half minutes to all be part of the same ingroup. For this brief time, we can ignore our political differences, our sports team allegiances, and our divisive family disagreements.

If you have the right class for it—before covering ingroups—play Y.M.C.A and encourage your students to dance along. To close your discussion of ingroups, point out how the dance created a temporary ingroup. Ask your students to work in small groups to identify other ways temporary ingroups can be created. Finally, if you haven’t yet, show your students the TV2 Denmark All That We Share video.

And that show that the ship’s crew performed? They included Y.M.C.A. Perhaps its an international dance.

Reference

Pearlman, J. (2008, May 27). “Y.M.C.A.” (An Oral History). Spin. https://www.spin.com/2008/05/ymca-oral-history/




The social psychology of viral eateries: A class discussion

I enjoy a good bagel, and I enjoy a news story that promises to illustrate several social psychological principles. When the headline reads “The Hole in This Bagel Shop’s Business Model? It’s Too Popular” (Balk, 2024), I’m in.

Apollo Bagels in New York City’s West village has become so popular that the lines stretch to 100 feet. And that was before the November 30, 2024 New York Times story.

After covering the social psychology chapter in Intro Psych, ask your students to read the story (the link is gifted to you, so your students do not need a New York Times account). And, then, as your students to work in small groups to answer these questions.

  1. One patron said, “If it’s gone viral on Instagram, I’ve stood in the line and waited for it.” Which of Robert Cialdini’s six principles of persuasion best account for this behavior: reciprocity, scarcity, authority, commitment and consistency, liking, consensus? Explain.
  2. The article states that there is a chance that Apollo’s Bagels will be evicted from this location. What does the persuasion principle of scarcity will predict will happen with the lines? Will they get longer, shorter, or no change? Explain.
  3. After standing in line, a patron declared that the bagel she purchased was “phenomenal.” From the story, we don’t know how long she waited in line, but let’s imagine she waited 20 minutes. Explain how cognitive dissonance could have made an objectively average bagel exceptionally good.
  4. In the comments to the article, Daneil Kfoury wrote on November 30, 2024, “I don’t love waiting in lines, but I am from Texas, and we wait in line for great BBQ. It’s part of the experience. It’s a wonderful opportunity to mingle with strangers you may never mix with and experience humanity.” Explain Kfoury’s comments in terms of ingroups.

For the AI-conscious, ChatGPT nailed questions 1, 2, 3, but whiffed on 4.

If you’d like to incorporate AI, ask students in their groups to use AI to answer the questions and then critique AI’s responses based on what they’ve learned in class about these concepts (Costa et al., 2024). These same instructions would also work as a solo take-home assignment.  

References

Balk, T. (2024, November 30). A hole in apollo bagels’ business model? It’s too popular. The New York Times. https://www.nytimes.com/2024/11/30/nyregion/apollo-bagels-nyc.html

Costa, C., Husain-Habib, N., & Reiter, A. (2024). Integrating ai into education: Successful strategies, ideas, and tools from psychology instructors. Teaching of Psychology, 00986283241297635. https://doi.org/10.1177/00986283241297635

[The attached photo is not of an Apollo Bagels bagel, as far as I know.]




Let’s talk about grief

My father died of cancer when I was a teenager. He was 52. I didn’t realize how young he was until I surpassed his age. My father died in middle age. It wasn’t his middle age.

I’ve never liked covering death and dying at the end of the Intro Psych lifespan chapter. By putting death after discussion of being an older adult, the implication is that that is the right time to die. Everyone who dies before their 80s or 90s has evidently done life wrong. That doesn’t sit well with me.

In my Intro Psych textbook, the previous author had placed death and dying at the end of lifespan. When I revised the book, I deleted that section of the chapter. That didn’t feel right, either. It’s an important topic that we need to be talking about.

I visited the booths at the 2024 American Psychological Association convention. I could claim that I enjoy seeing what’s new in the land of vendors, but really, I just like seeing what’s new in promo items. My favorites these days are branded Tide pens and neck gaiters. I also like lip balm, but I may have a lifetime supply now. Even if my lifetime extends deep into my 90s.

On this particular visit to the vendor booths, I met Jen Reising and Heather Taylor of The Mourning Movement who offer “professional grief resources to build community and change the narrative around grief.”

In my conversation with Jen and Heather, the death and dying coverage solution came to me. Our Intro Psych coverage should not be about death and dying but about grief. Yes, we experience grief from the death of someone we love—including a pet—or our own impending death, but we also experience grief that results from other losses such as a job, a friendship or other relationship. A presidential election. Each requires coping with the loss of a previously imagined future.

Jen and Heather write:

Grief is not just about sadness; it encompasses a myriad of feelings that can surprise us in their intensity and variety. You might find yourself oscillating between anger and relief, guilt and nostalgia, or despair and love—all within a short span of time. This is perfectly normal. Emotions in grief are like colors in a palette, each adding depth and nuance to our experience of loss (Reising & Taylor, 2024).

Rather than cover death and dying in the lifespan chapter, I’m going to cover the more global feeling of grief in the emotions chapter.

In a survey of 800 Intro Psych instructors, one-third reported that they don’t cover emotion (Richmond et al., 2021). Some instructors who exclude emotion have told me that discussion of the theories of emotion bores them. What if we changed the focus of the emotions chapter from theories that explain where emotions come from to focus on some of our more powerful emotions? Let’s talk about happiness and anger. And, yes, let’s also talk about the complex emotions that come with grief.

References

Reising, J., & Taylor, H. (2024, September 11). The many faces of grief—Embracing emotional diversity. The  Mourning  Movement. https://themourningmovement.com/f/the-many-faces-of-grief—embracing-emotional-diversity

Richmond, A. S., Boysen, G. A., Hudson, D. L., Gurung, R. A. R., Naufel, K. Z., Neufeld, G., Landrum, R. E., Dunn, D. S., & Beers, M. (2021). The Introductory Psychology census: A national study. Scholarship of Teaching and Learning in Psychology, 7(3), 163–180. https://doi.org/10.1037/stl0000277




What if it’s not about the grades?

The 2024-2025 ski racing season is underway. Mikaela Shiffrin, at the age of 29 holds the record for the most World Cup alpine skiing wins with 97. She blew past the previous record holder, Ingemar Stenmark, who had 86 wins. Stenmark’s record was set in 1989 and was thought to be unbeatable.

In alpine skiing, winners are commonly determined by tenths of a second. In a recent race, less than a second and a half separated the top eight racers (Zaccardi, 2024). All racers work to improve their technique in order to improve their speed. Their goal is not to win; their goal is to find a faster way down the mountain. Mikaela Shiffrin is no different from other top athletes in that way. Get faster; the wins will come (Armour, 2024).

That’s a great example of the difference between extrinsic and intrinsic motivation. Are you working for the glory and the praise that comes with a win? Then you are extrinsically motivated. Or are you working to get better for the sake of getting better? Do you want to be the best you that you can be? Then you are intrinsically motivated.

Ask your students—in a free response format, index cards work fine—“Why are you in college?” (If you teach high school students, ask what they plan to do after high school, and then ask them why.)

For each card, read it silently to yourself to make sure the content is appropriate, then read it out loud. Ask your students if the motivation sounds more intrinsic, more extrinsic, or if there isn’t enough information to make a determination. In the case of college, if a student says they’re here to get a degree or to qualify for a job, their motivation is extrinsic. However, if they say they’re here to learn, their motivation is intrinsic.

Of course, this is all a bit more nuanced. I’ve had students tell me directly that they don’t care about my course because they’re not psychology majors. They were much more intrinsically motivated in courses that had prefixes that matched their major. My thought process after that was pretty straight-forward. If my students aren’t seeing why psychology matters to them, then I need to make it clear why psychology should matter to them. If I couldn’t explain why particular course content should matter, that course content got tossed.

Who is more likely to cheat? Those who are extrinsically motivated (Krou et al., 2021). A student who just wants the degree can cheat their way through college and get that degree without learning.

A student who wants to learn will do the work required for learning. The grades—and the degree—will follow as a natural byproduct. Just as the wins follow for skiers who work to get better.  

Here’s the million dollar question. How do we help students shift from extrinsic motivation to intrinsic motivation?

Shiffrin’s 97th victory

References

Armour, N. (2024, October 25). Mikaela Shiffrin is using her records pursuit for good. USA Today. https://www.usatoday.com/story/sports/columnist/nancy-armour/2024/10/25/mikaela-shiffrin-world-cup-milestone/75816994007/

Krou, M. R., Fong, C. J., & Hoff, M. A. (2021). Achievement motivation and academic dishonesty: A meta-analytic investigation. Educational Psychology Review, 33(2), 427–458. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-020-09557-7

Zaccardi, N. (2024, October 26). Federica Brignone becomes oldest woman to win Alpine skiing World Cup race. NBC Sports. https://www.nbcsports.com/olympics/news/federica-brignone-mikaela-shiffrin-alpine-skiing-world-cup-soelden




“Crazy” pizza commercial: Discussion of stigma or observational learning

It’s another fall Sunday in the U.S., so it’s another day of TV commercials featuring NFL players.

I know that being a middle-aged woman does not make me the target demographic, but the Little Caesars ad featuring George Kittle still rankles.

Let me get this out of the way first. Yes, that is Caesars plural, not Caesar’s possessive. I can make peace with that. I can believe that there are a bunch of little Caesars, not just one who has pizza.

Now for the commercial. Give it a watch.

Again, I recognize I’m not the demographic, but I don’t understand how watching 11 people (including a garden gnome, a groundhog, and Kittle’s own reflection in the window) bully him through exclusion (“Everyone but you!”), is supposed to make me want me to buy Little Caesars crazy puffs. Probably they just want me to remember “crazy puffs,” and hope the buying follows from remembering.

Let’s discuss.

Little Caesars is implying that Kittle is “crazy,” since the product they’re hawking is “crazy puffs.” Everyone—including a garden gnome, a groundhog, and his own reflection—saying “everyone but you” and laughing is meant to imply that Kittle is experiencing paranoia.

This commercial could be an interesting discussion starter to open your coverage of psychological disorders. What do students think of Little Caesars using paranoia to sell pizza? Do the attitudes of your students differ between those who have been diagnosed with a psychological disorder or who have loved ones who have been and those who have not? Could the commercial further the stigma for those who have been diagnosed with a psychological disorder? Why or why not?

The commercial rankles for another reason—making it a good discussion starter for the learning chapter.  We know observational learning is powerful.  Will people—particularly children—start saying “everyone but you!”? It’s very easy to see how this could cross the line from an inside joke amongst family or friends to bullying. In the comments for the YouTube video, one person wrote, “This is now the favorite phrase of my household.” I can’t help but picture a child who is a frequent target of their siblings’ bullying now being hit with “Everyone but you!”

Lastly, ask your students if advertisers have a responsibility to consider possible unintentional outcomes of their commercials. Why or why not? If you would like to expand this discussion with some what-ifs, ask students to consider what Kittle might have done after we see him scream at the end of the commercial. Write student responses where everyone can see them. For each one, ask students what if Little Caesars had showed that outcome? Would it have made the commercial more acceptable to air, less acceptable to air, or no change? Why?




How many of our students have cannabinoid hyperemesis syndrome?

An estimated 43.6 million people in the U.S. who are 12 or older—that’s 15.4%—reported using marijuana in the last month. The largest group of people who used in the last month? They would be 18 to 25 year olds; 25.2% (8.6 million or so) (Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, 2024a).

How about daily use? In 2023, an estimated 15.7 million people in the U.S. who are 12 or older—that’s 5.6%—reported using marijuana daily or almost daily. The largest group of daily users? Yes, that would be 18 to 25 year olds again; 9.2% (3.1 million or so) (Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, 2024b).

These data come from the 2023 National Survey on Drug Use and Health (NSDUH). “NSDUH employs a probability sample designed to be representative of both the nation as a whole and for each of the 50 states and the District of Columbia” (Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, 2024a, p. 5). If you are not familiar with how NSDUH (pronounced nizz-duh) surveys nearly 70,000 people every year, check out their methodological summary (Center for Behavioral Health Statistics and Quality, 2024).

Side note. For about a year, my wife Verla worked as one of hundreds of people who went to the homes that had been randomly selected for the NSDUH survey. The homes were sent a letter in advance, so they could expect her. Once she made contact with someone in the home, she would ask for basic demographic information for each member of the household. After entering that into a tablet-like device, she would press a button, and the device would select zero, one, or two people from the household to complete the survey. If one or more of the selected people weren’t at home, she would have to come back. She would set up the survey on a laptop, and then situate herself so that the person could complete the survey in privacy. Every effort was made to reach the people who had been randomly selected.

While NSDUH does not identify whether or not a person is a college student, the annual Monitoring the Future (MTF) survey does. Interestingly, the MTF notes, “The term ‘marijuana’ is increasingly being replaced with the term ‘cannabis.’ In our surveys, we now use both terms” (Patrick et al., 2024, p. 27). Their 2023 survey results have 10.4% of young adults (defined as ages19-30) reporting daily or almost daily use (at least 20 days of use in the last 30 days) of marijuana. That 10.4% is right in line with the 9.2% (ages 18-25) of daily/almost daily use that the NSDUH survey found.

Now, what about (traditional) college students? The MTF asks people between the ages of 19 and 22 if they are a full-time college student. Nonstudents, then, is anyone in that same age range who is not attending college full-time. The 19- to 22-year-olds who reported using marijuana monthly did not differ much between those who attended college full-time (26.1%) and those who didn’t (28.8%). Daily use, however, showed a notable difference: full-time college students (6.3%) and those who aren’t (11.6%).

Discussion question for your students: Why do you think there is this difference in daily/almost daily use of marijuana/cannabis between 19- to 22-year-olds who are full-time college students and those who are not?

[My first thought was that it would be tough to keep grades up if one were stoned all the time. My second thought was that smoking or vaping pot in the dorms would likely get one kicked out of college. But edibles?]

Here’s the problem. Recreational cannabis is, as of October 2024, legal in 24 states (Breen & Johnston, 2024) which makes it easier to get. At the same time, cannabis has become more potent (ElSohly et al., 2021).  

From the NSDUH survey, 16.6% of 18- to 25-year-olds met the criteria for cannabis use disorder at some point in the previous year (Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, 2024a). Here are the DSM-5 criteria for cannabis use disorder and the ICD-11 criteria for cannabis dependence.

What many cannabis users don’t know is that frequent cannabis use can cause cannabinoid hyperemesis syndrome (CHS). CHS “is an enigmatic constellation of signs and symptoms comprising nausea, vomiting, abdominal pain, and unusual hot bathing behavior in the context of heavy and chronic exposure to tetrahydrocannabinol (THC)” (Russo et al., 2022, p. 336). Hot bathing? For reasons that are not yet clear, heat relieves the symptoms of vomiting, nausea, and abdominal pain. One hypothesis is that since there are cannabinoid receptors in the hypothalamus, the heat resets any sort of dysregulation happening there. Another hypothesis is that since heat causes vasodilation of the blood vessels in the skin, blood moves away from the abdomen, relieving pressure on the digestive tract (Ali et al., 2021). However, these are not the only possibilities (Veloso et al., 2024).  

People have died from CHS (Nourbakhsh et al., 2019; Rudavsky, 2019). Because some physicians may not know to look for CHS and because cannabis users are reluctant to believe that cannabis is the cause, it is likely underdiagnosed. In this study, 87% of participants stopped having symptoms when they stopped using cannabis, “most suffering recurrence rapidly after resumption” (Russo et al., 2022, p. 336).

Emergency department physicians, however, have begun seeing CHS much more frequently (Twohey et al., 2024) (gifted New York Times article).  

Here’s a different problem. Longitudinal research has found that teenagers who use marijuana are at an increased risk of developing schizophrenia (Godin & Shehata, 2022). People who are genetically predisposed to developing schizophrenia are at even greater risk (Wainberg et al., 2021).

And, yes, cannabis users can become dependent on it, particularly those who started using it to relieve anxiety or depression (Twohey et al., 2024).

 Invite students to read this gifted New York Times article.

Twohey, M., Ivory, D., & Kessler, C. (2024, October 4). As America’s marijuana use grows, so do the harms. The New York Times. https://www.nytimes.com/2024/10/04/us/cannabis-marijuana-risks-addiction.html

Discussion prompts:

  1. What was the most surprising thing you learned from reading this article? Explain.
  2. What is the one thing from this article you want your family and friends to know? Explain.

References

Ali, A. A., Mathew, R., Maaliki, N., & Shelley, P. (2021). Recurring vomiting, skin discolouration: The easily overlooked thermal element of cannabinoid hyperemesis syndrome. BMJ Case Reports, 14(3), e242062. https://doi.org/10.1136/bcr-2021-242062

Breen, K., & Johnston, T. (2024, May 17). Maps show states where weed is legal for recreational, medical use in 2024. CBS News. https://www.cbsnews.com/news/legal-weed-map-states/

Center for Behavioral Health Statistics and Quality. (2024). 2023 National Survey on Drug Use and Health (NSDUH): Methodological summary and definitions. Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. https://www.samhsa.gov/data/report/2023-methodological-summary-and-definitions

ElSohly, M. A., Chandra, S., Radwan, M., Majumdar, C. G., & Church, J. C. (2021). A comprehensive review of cannabis potency in the United States in the last decade. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 6(6), 603–606. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2020.12.016

Godin, S., & Shehata, S. (2022). Adolescent cannabis use and later development of schizophrenia: An updated systematic review of longitudinal studies. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 78(7), 1331–1340. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.23312

Nourbakhsh, M., Miller, A., Gofton, J., Jones, G., & Adeagbo, B. (2019). Cannabinoid hyperemesis syndrome: Reports of fatal cases. Journal of Forensic Sciences, 64(1), 270–274. https://doi.org/10.1111/1556-4029.13819

Patrick, M. E., Miech, R. A., Johnston, L. D., & O’Malley, P. M. (2024). Monitoring the Future Panel Study annual report: National data on substance use among adults ages 19 to 65, 1976-2023 (Monitoring the Future Monograph Series). Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan. https://monitoringthefuture.org/results/annual-reports/

Rudavsky, S. (2019, September 20). He loved weed. Then the vomiting began. Months later, he died. USA Today. https://www.usatoday.com/story/news/nation/2019/09/20/indiana-boy-17-died-smoking-weed-chs-blame-what-chs/2387571001/

Russo, E. B., Spooner, C., May, L., Leslie, R., & Whiteley, V. L. (2022). Cannabinoid hyperemesis syndrome survey and genomic investigation. Cannabis and Cannabinoid Research, 7(3), 336–344. https://doi.org/10.1089/can.2021.0046

Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. (2024a). Key substance use and mental health indicators in the United States: Results from the 2023 National Survey on Drug Use and Health (HHS Publication No. PEP24-07-021, NSDUH Series H-59)). Center for Behavioral Health Statistics and Quality, Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. https://www.samhsa.gov/data/report/2023-nsduh-annual-national-report

Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. (2024b). Section 8 PE Tables—Results from the 2023 National Survey on Drug Use and Health: Detailed Tables, SAMHSA, CBHSQ. Center for Behavioral Health Statistics and Quality, Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. https://www.samhsa.gov/data/sites/default/files/reports/rpt47100/NSDUHDetailedTabs2023/NSDUHDetailedTabs2023/2023-nsduh-detailed-tables-sect8pe.htm

Twohey, M., Ivory, D., & Kessler, C. (2024, October 4). As America’s marijuana use grows, so do the harms. The New York Times. https://www.nytimes.com/2024/10/04/us/cannabis-marijuana-risks-addiction.html

Veloso, P., Gomes, M., Lopes De Dios, R., & Pereira, F. (2024). Cannabinoid hyperemesis syndrome: A case report and literature review. European Psychiatry, 67(S1), S409–S410. https://doi.org/10.1192/j.eurpsy.2024.844

Wainberg, M., Jacobs, G. R., Di Forti, M., & Tripathy, S. J. (2021). Cannabis, schizophrenia genetic risk, and psychotic experiences: A cross-sectional study of 109,308 participants from the UK Biobank. Translational Psychiatry, 11(1), 211. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-021-01330-w




Psych-related news scrolling on the right

On the right side of this blog site is an area called Psych-Related News. Scroll down for the most recent news.

In my own news feed, I scroll through a few hundred articles (and 30+ comic strips) every day. When I see an interesting article (or comic strip) that may be appropriate for one of my book chapters or for this blog, I tag it. It’s the most recent 15 of those tagged articles (or comic strips) that appear in the Psych-Related News area.

If you have your own news feed reader (I use Inoreader) and would like read the Psych-Related News at your leisure, here is the RSS feed address: https://www.inoreader.com/stream/user/1005880865/tag/Psych_Matters_blog. Any time I tag a new article (or comic strip!) as Psych-Related News, you will see it pop up in your news feed.

If that all seems like too much work, just visit SueFrantz.com whenever you’d like and scroll through the Psych-Related News. New content is added once or twice a day, most commonly first thing in the morning mountain time.




Emotion regulation: Discussion about Herb and Jamaal (May 12, 2024)

Cartoonists are often astute observers of people. Since cartoonists usually don’t name the psychological concept that they are illustrating, their cartoons can provide excellent fodder for class discussion.

Before we get to the discussion, note that the comic strip below is part of the Go Comics family which uses Andrews McMeel licensing. Be sure to read their classroom usage statement to ensure that you do not inadvertently violate copyright law.

Before beginning this activity, make your students understand the different types of emotion regulation (McRae & Gross, 2020).

Situation selection. We select a particular situation in order to experience a particular emotion. For example, if we want to experience the excitement of fear, we might watch a horror movie. Okay, no, I would never do that. I learned a long, long time ago, that horror movies are not my thing. But other people certainly do that. (Ask your students to raise their hand if they enjoy horror movies. And then email me [sue@suefrantz.com] with what you learn.)

Situation modification. When we are stuck with a particular situation, we may be able to modify it in some way that would alter our emotions. Let’s say that we find ourselves watching a horror movie—perhaps because our friends talked us into it—we might choose to put in ear plugs to reduce the sound.

Attentional deployment. Where we put our attention can influence our emotions. At the horror movie, we can shift our attention by putting in earbuds, closing our eyes, and listening to a favorite podcast.

Cognitive change. Here we change how we think about the situation we are in. For the horror movie, shifting from “movie watcher” to “movie critic” may help reduce fear. If we think about the quality of the story, the acting, the makeup, the scenery, and the sound, our experience of the movie may shift from being part of it to being removed from it.

Response modulation. Imagine that we are full blown in the middle of feeling fear during this horror movie. We can reduce the intensity of the fear by doing things like taking deep breaths and relaxing our muscles.

With that common understanding of emotion regulation, let’s go to the cartoon!

Ask students to read it: Herb and Jamaal cartoon dated Sunday, May 12, 2024

Working in small groups, give students these two discussion prompts:

  1. Give an example of how the son could use each of the five emotion regulation strategies to reduce his anger.
  2. Is the son’s father giving advice about a particular emotion regulation strategy? Explain.

To conclude the activity, ask volunteers from each group to share examples they generated for situation selection. After each group has offered one example, repeat with situation modification, and so on through all five strategies. Finally, ask volunteers from each group to offer their thoughts on the father’s advice. Is he advising his son to use a specific emotion regulation strategy or just some emotion regulation strategy?

Reference

McRae, K., & Gross, J. J. (2020). Emotion regulation. Emotion, 20(1), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000703